ABSTRACT
Although various types of price promotion are used to increase sales, they negatively may affect consumers' perceptions of a premium-product brand. The authors of this study believe their work advances the limited research in this area by distinguishing between direct- versus indirect-price reduction and marketers' use of a “precondition”—i.e., promotions offering free gifts, trade-in incentives, or loyalty-program benefits. Results of this two-part study, which combined sales data of German premium automobile brands with consumer-behavior analysis, showed that direct-price reduction had the strongest positive sales impact. Brand perception was least deteriorated by both direct-price reduction without a precondition—and indirect-price reduction with a precondition.
MANAGEMENT SLANT
Different price-promotion types have different influences on sales and brand perception in the premium automotive market.
Direct-price reduction has the strongest positive impact on sales numbers.
Price-promotion types with which the customer is not familiar may be harmful for the brand.
Management should focus on price-promotion types—with direct-price reduction— that also are familiar to the customer.
INTRODUCTION
Price promotion represents one of the instruments most frequently used by marketing and sales managers to increase sales (e.g., Blattberg and Neslin, 1990; Low and Mohr, 1999). It is characterized either by reducing the price of a product or enriching the product while keeping the price constant. Customers perceive an economic advantage when purchasing the promoted product (Raghubir and Corfman, 1999). The positive sales impact, however, oftentimes comes with detrimental effects on the brand (Montaner and Pina, 2008).
There is a rich tradition of scholarly work on sales promotions, in general, and price promotions, in particular (e.g., Ehrenberg and Scriven, 1997; Gupta, 1988). Although the general positive sales effects of price promotions thoroughly have been investigated (e.g., Blattberg, Eppen, and Lieberman, 1981; Gupta, 1988; Thompson and Noordewier, 1992; Woodside and Waddle, 1975), the negative effects of such programs on a brand (Martinez, Montaner, and Pina, 2006; Yoo, Donthu, and Lee, 2000) may be underestimated (Blattberg and Neslin, 1990). Despite different arguments about the positive and negative effects of price promotion, current knowledge is based on diverse objects of research and isolated studies that focus on sales impact or brand perception only.
Furthermore, previous research often has considered price promotion in general (e.g., Villarejo-Ramos and Sánchez-Franco, 2005; Yoo et al., 2000) or concentrated on one or two specific types (e.g., Raghubir, 2004; Beltramini and Chapman, 2003). Understanding brand-perception effects of price promotion as well as sales, however, is vital for companies that heavily rely on brands, especially manufacturers of premium products.
Compared with products that target mass markets, premium products are of higher quality, are selectively distributed, and have a higher price position (Quelch, Neslin, and Olson, 1987). Moreover, premium products differ from luxury products. Indeed, luxury products
rely even more on a brand;
pursue an exclusive distribution strategy;
are scarce with limited supply; and
are able to command even higher prices, as the latter are used as a signal of a luxury product's exclusivity (Christodoulides, Michaelidou, and Li, 2008; Truong, McColl, and Kitchen, 2009).
An example of a luxury fashion brand is Louis Vuitton and a luxury mobile phone brand is Vertu, whereas a premium fashion brand is Hugo Boss and a premium mobile phone product is Apple's iPhone. In addition, whereas price promotion for mass-market products represents a frequently applied marketing instrument, prices for luxury products rarely are reduced (Keller, 2009).
For premium brands, however, an increasing use of different price promotions can be observed (Edmunds.com, 2010). In addition to direct discounts, for example, marketers of premium products frequently frame price promotions differently—such as by offering a free gift with purchase promotion—to hide the discount and thereby decrease the perception of brand deterioration (Raghubir, 2004). The duality of sales impact and brand impact of different price-promotion activities thus gains in relevance.
Despite the high relevance of analyzing sales impact and brand-perception impact for premium products, the current authors believe that previous studies have not investigated this topic sufficiently. The current research contributes to the existing literature by
proposing a framework that distinguishes four different price-promotion types;
consistently investigating sales, as well as how these specific price-promotion activities affect consumers' perceptions of the brand.
Thus, the authors believe they have expanded existing knowledge about price promotion with a more differentiated investigation and specifically by considering the case of premium products.
LITERATURE REVIEW
Price Promotions' Impact on Sales
Marketers use price-promotion activities as one of the key tools to increase sales and turnover (e.g., Alvarez and Casielles, 2005). Various studies have found an influence of price promotion on sales (e.g., Blattberg et al., 1981; Gupta, 1988). The existing research can be divided into two categories:
studies limited to self-reported data, mainly suffering from analyses of mere purchase intentions instead of real behavior (e.g., Alvarez and Casielles, 2005), and
analyses of actual market data (e.g., Silva-Risso and Ionova, 2008).
Furthermore, the existing scholarship varies in the types of products (“durable” versus “nondurable” goods) that are investigated (Spears, 2001). Specific characteristics of durable and nondurable products (such as different lead times and different unit prices) lead to diverse promotion types and different percentage discount rates (Quelch et al., 1987; Thompson and Noordewier, 1992).
The majority of promotion research has focused on nondurable products (e.g., Chen, Monroe, and Lou, 1998; Gupta, 1988; Raghubir, 2004); with some exceptions (Silva-Risso and Ionova, 2008; Beltramini and Chapman, 2003) investigating price promotion in the automobile industry. One analysis of the sales impact of promotion activities—based on real-market data from the automotive industry—found that a sales impact of price promotion (in this case, price discount) does not always occur (Thompson and Noordewier, 1992), but it did not take into account the different types of promotion activities (See Appendix A on page 283).
Price Promotions' Impact On Brand Perception
Although price promotion generally is regarded as an efficient technique for short-term increases in sales, it negatively can affect a brand's reputation in the long run (Blattberg and Neslin, 1989; Martinez et al., 2006). Negative effects that are referred to in the literature include (Blattberg and Neslin, 1989; Del Vecchio, Henard, and Freling, 2006):
decreased brand preference;
lower brand loyalty;
decreased quality perception and brand image (Blattberg and Neslin, 1989; Del Vecchio et al., 2006).
There have been mixed results in the analysis of the impact of sales promotion on brand perception. Some scholars have revealed negative results (e.g., Martinez et al., 2006; Yoo et al., 2000). Still others could not find negative effects for the brand (Davis, Inman, and McAlister, 1992; Raghubir and Corfman, 1999). These differences in the findings can be explained by the product category and the promotion occurrence. Negative effects are lower when
the consumer involvement with the product purchase is low,
the product provides a low utilitarian value for the customer, or
when the common nature of sales promotion for the brand or in the respective industry is low (Davis et al., 1992; Raghubir and Corfman, 1999).
The current study focused on price promotion for premium products—specifically premium automobiles—that represent high-involvement durable goods at high prices, positioned as high-quality products (Quelch, 1987). The high price is an important point of differentiation between a premium brand and a mid-range brand and, thus, can be one factor for establishing premium-brand reputation (Quelch, 1987; Truong et al., 2009). Price promotion, thus, often can be assumed to be harmful for premium automotive brands (Blattberg and Neslin, 1990). It can be assumed that it would be even more harmful for luxury brands, but those brands usually do not use price promotion (Christodoulides et al., 2008; Truong et al., 2009) and are not in the scope of the current research.
Most analyses of brand effects of price promotion have considered perceived quality and/or brand image (e.g., Darke and Chung, 2005; Villarejo-Ramos and Sánchez-Franco, 2005; Yoo et al., 2000). In the case of premium products, however, the price also has signaled prestige, representing a key aspect for the perception of a premium brand (Keller, 2009; Truong et al., 2009).
Discounting the price, therefore, may exert a negative influence on prestige, leading to a decline in brand perception, which would be even more detrimental for premium brands compared with mass-market brands. Thus, the current authors believe, when investigating the impact of price promotion on brand reputation of premium products, brand image as well as prestige both should be considered.
HYPOTHESES DEVELOPMENT
Types of Price-Promotion Activities
Marketers commonly use a broad variety of price-promotion activities, such as:
direct-price discounts that lower the selling price in a specific time period (Alvarez and Casielles, 2005; Hardesty and Bearden, 2003; Silva-Risso and Ionova, 2008);
free-gift promotion, where the price remains constant, but the product value is enriched (Laroche et al., 2003; Hardesty and Bearden, 2003; Weisstein, Monroe, and Kukar-Kinney, 2013).
These price-promotion activities are distinguished by the following two criteria:
the “directness” of the price reduction. In the case of direct-price discounts, a price-promotion activity can offer a direct discount to a product's list price. For free-gift promotions, an indirect-price reduction can be offered, where the price remains constant but the purchase matter is extended, for instance, by adding specific products or components.
The current study's proposed framework thus distinguishes between direct- and indirect-price-promotion activities.
whether they apply to all purchases or only if the customer meets a certain precondition. Some promotions, for instance, only are applicable if an older product is traded in. Other activities only apply for new or existing customers.
This second criterion that is used in the proposed framework thus is the existence of a precondition.
Although other characteristics—a promotion activity's relative discount, for instance—also may influence consumers' reactions, these factors are market and product specific. The proposed framework, however, is based on two universal, overarching characteristics (directness of price reduction and existence of a precondition) and thus allows for delineating the majority of existing price-promotion activities.
Applying this framework, four types of price-promotion activities can be distinguished (See Figure 1):
Type 1: direct-price reduction, without any precondition, such as direct discount;
Type 2: indirect-price reduction, without any precondition, such as free-gift promotions. In the consumer electronics industry, for instance, camera manufacturers offer free equipment (e.g., lens) with product purchase;
Type 3: direct-price reduction, with a precondition, for example, a loyalty promotion that only applies to existing customers.
Type 4: indirect-price reduction that requires a precondition. For example, a customer might trade in his old car—the necessary precondition. Instead of receiving a direct discount on the new car sales price, however, he receives higher compensation for his old car and, thus, an indirect-price reduction. As another example, a premium bicycle manufacturer could implement the trade-in of an old bicycle as a promotion for the customer.
Sales Impact of Different Types Of Price Promotions
To explain customers' reactions to price-promotion activities, prospect theory (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979) frequently is used. The theory proposes that losses are perceived as greater-than-equal gains, as the negative effect of a loss is perceived to be greater than the positive effect of an equal gain (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979). This underlying effect also is applicable for the distinction between the four categories of price-promotion activities.
Specifically, the price that a customer has to pay for a product represents a loss, whereas obtaining the product represents a gain.
Transactions with a direct-price reduction, therefore, decrease the loss while the gain (i.e., the product) remains constant. Indirect-price reduction, on the other hand, does not affect the loss (i.e., the price to be paid) but increases the gain, as the customer receives more for the same price. Assuming an equal monetary value of each promotion type, a direct-price reduction thus would be perceived as more positive than an indirect-price reduction and, therefore, should have a significantly stronger sales impact.
H1: Direct-price reduction has a stronger positive sales impact than indirect-price reduction of the same value.
From a customer's point of view, price promotions with a precondition may require additional time and effort to receive the promotion benefit. As customers generally value time and effort (Seiders, Berry, and Gresham, 2000), additional actions are necessary before a purchase can be assumed to decrease the value of a promotion activity. This effect has been shown with coupons for nondurable goods: The effort for the redemption of the coupons led to lower purchase intentions compared with other promotion tools (Del Vecchio et al., 2006).
Considering the gains and losses incurred by a price-promotion activity, the introduction of a precondition increases the loss—i.e., the time and effort required for meeting the precondition in addition to the price. When considering this assumed attenuating effect of a precondition, however, it is important to differentiate between direct- and indirect-price reduction.
A direct-price reduction makes it easy for customers to quantify the value of the time and effort required to meet the precondition. For instance, redeeming a coupon to receive $1 directly can be related to the aggravation of remembering to take the coupon along and present it on checkout. Compared with direct-price reduction without a precondition, those activities that restrict customers' eligibility, thus, can be assumed to have a lower sales impact.
For indirect-price reduction, however, such quantifications are far more difficult. Receiving additional products or product components for the same price does not allow for a direct assessment of the monetary value of the time and effort required for meeting the precondition. It therefore can be assumed that the attenuating effect of a precondition will not occur for indirect-price reduction.
H2: The introduction of a precondition attenuates the sales impact of direct-price reduction, but it does not influence the sales impact of indirect-price reduction.
Different effects between direct- and indirect-price reduction on nondurable products have been investigated previously. Direct-price reduction seems to be the most effective price-reduction method for increasing volume (Alvarez and Casielles, 2005; Neslin, Henderson, and Quelch, 1985).
The current authors also expected a precondition to impact sales differently, considering previous investigation of different effects of, for instance, gift cards and free gifts on high- versus low-priced products (Weisstein et al., 2013).
For high-priced products, gift cards were found to be more effective than free gifts, whereas the opposite result was obtained for low-priced products (Weisstein et al., 2013):
Customers would perceive redeeming the gift card as less annoying when the reward was higher (i.e., high-priced product), but
customers would perceive redeeming a gift card for a low-priced product an annoyance (Weisstein et al., 2013).
Indeed, for low-priced products, the reward was deemed not to be worth the aggravation of redeeming the gift card. Meeting a precondition requires time and effort by the customer and, thus, can be related to the gift-card example. When promoting a highly priced premium product, the annoyance of meeting a precondition is small compared with the monetary value of the promotion.
Combining the aforementioned theoretical rationale, the authors believe it can be assumed that, for premium products, the directness of the price reduction has a stronger impact on sales than the nonexistence of a precondition.
H3: The directness of a price reduction has a stronger positive sales impact than the nonexistence of a precondition.
Brand-Perception Impact of Different Price-Promotion Types
Changes in customers' brand perception because of price promotion can be explained by attribution theory, which states that individuals search for causal reasons of actions (Kelley, 1973; Mizerski, Golden, and Kernan, 1979).
When trying to justify price promotions, consumers may assume negative brand issues as reasons for a company not being able to sell the product at a regular price (Blattberg and Neslin, 1990). Because premium manufacturers heavily rely on brands, the authors of this article considered the general brand image (Low and Lamb, 2000) as well as a brand's prestige (Baek, Kim, and Yu, 2010).
Although brand image is a commonly investigated construct (e.g., Martinez et al., 2006; Montaner and Pina, 2008), prestige has not been considered in the context of price promotion. Prestige is an outcome of a premium brand's high-price position and represents the product's high status (Baek et al., 2010) and, therefore, is a key attribute of premium brands (Keller, 2009; Truong et al., 2009).
In contrast to the premium-specific brand-attribute prestige, which is based on the high-price position, the general brand image is represented by a set of associations in the mind of a consumer and is built through different advertising and marketing activities (Aaker, 1991; Dobni and Zinkhan, 1990). Overall, price-promotion activities are assumed to lower consumers' brand perception in both constructs.
H4a: Price-promotion activities lead to a decrease in brand image.
H4b: Price-promotion activities lead to a decrease in prestige.
Additionally, consumers may perceive each price-promotion type differently, leading to a differentiated impact on brand perception. Extending attribution theory, the covariation principle proposes that individuals use patterns to build such attributions (Kelley, 1973). Only if actions and results can be observed over time (i.e., are common and/or frequent), attributions can be made with certainty (Kelley, 1973; Mizerski et al., 1979).
This leads to the assumption that if consumers are used to a certain type of promotion, causal attributions regarding negative brand reputation would be less likely than if the type of promotion is regarded as unusual. For a price-promotion activity to prevent negative brand effects, it should be perceived as relatively common. Considering that consumers will evaluate each price-promotion type on how common it is in a market, the negative attributions should vary in their intensity for the different price-promotion types.
The authors of the current study, therefore, felt it was relevant to consider the occurrence of the four different types of price-promotion activities in a market and their relationship with one another, to evaluate the specific brand effects:
Type 1: Direct discount (direct-price reduction without precondition) was regarded as the typical type of price promotion broadly used for nondurable products as well as, for instance, automobiles and consumer electronics (e.g., Al-Sibai and Hofer, 2004; Darke and Chung, 2005). The high occurrence of this type of promotion, in comparison to other types, was informed by descriptive data of the first study with market data (See Table 2).
Type 4: Indirect-price reduction with a precondition was not represented as often as Type 1 (See Table 2), because premium manufacturers do not use this tool broadly. In contrast, mass-market automotive manufacturers may use indirect-price reduction with a precondition more frequently (Al-Sibai and Hofer, 2004; Diez, 2006). Because mass-market automobile manufacturers communicate their promotion activities more often, it can be assumed that consumers regard indirect-price reduction with a precondition as common for automobile companies.
Type 2: Indirect-price reduction without a precondition was described as being not as common as the two previous activities (Diez, 2006).
Type 3: Direct-price reduction with a precondition was not mentioned in the literature but occasionally has been used by premium automotive brands and thus is considered “unusual among auto incentive programs.”1
As Types 1 and 4 are most common (Al-Sibai and Hofer, 2004; Diez, 2006), it can be assumed that customers are familiar with these activities. On the basis of attribution theory and the covariation principle, these price-promotion types should be less detrimental for a brand than the more uncommon activities of “indirect-price reduction without a precondition” and “direct-price reduction with a precondition.”
H5a: “Direct-price reduction without a precondition” (Type 1) is less detrimental for brand image than “indirect-price reduction without a precondition” (Type 2).
H5b: “Direct-price reduction without a precondition” is less detrimental for prestige than “indirect-price reduction without a precondition.”
H6a: “Direct-price reduction without a precondition” (Type 1) is less detrimental for brand image than “direct-price reduction with a precondition” (Type 3).
H6b: “Direct-price reduction without a precondition” is less detrimental for prestige than “direct-price reduction with a precondition.”
H7a: “Indirect-price reduction with a precondition” (Type 4) is less detrimental for brand image than “indirect-price reduction without a precondition” (Type 2).
H7b: “Indirect-price reduction with a precondition” is less detrimental for prestige than “indirect-price reduction without a precondition.”
H8a: “Indirect-price reduction with a precondition” (Type 4) is less detrimental for brand image than “direct-price reduction with a precondition” (Type 3).
H8b: “Indirect-price reduction with a precondition” is less detrimental for prestige than “direct-price reduction with a precondition.”
STUDY 1: SALES IMPACT
The Data
Whereas much previous research has relied on self-reported data, the current study's hypotheses testing was based on the analysis of actual market data from a German premium car manufacturer. Generally, premium car brands—that is, Audi, BMW, Mercedes, and Lexus—are high priced and high quality.
The investigation, thus, represented a quasiexperiment (Campbell, 1969). Using such data requires consideration of the following two characteristics:
The majority of mass-market automobiles are built to stock. In contrast, the majority of premium cars are built to order, as customers choose their vehicles—with their specific configuration—which only then are manufactured. As customers consider very different car configurations, the purchase decision process is longer.
Price-promotion activities for premium cars thus usually last for about three months, compared with only one- or two-week periods for mass-market cars (Thompson and Noordewier, 1992).
The dataset analyzed in this study consisted of price-promotion activities that a car manufacturer had conducted between 2004 and 2010. In a first step, each promotion activity in a specific time period was classified to a category of the proposed price-promotion types (i.e., direct- versus indirect-price reduction; with versus without precondition promotions). Furthermore, the authors were able to secure information about
the sales of the promoted product;
the relative price discount;
the number of cars sold with the promotion;
competitors' market launches during the promotion period;
extent of push advertising campaigns;
occurrence of parallel promotion activities.
To minimize life-cycle effects, the current researchers only included data from products that had been on the market for at least one year before the start of a promotion activity and were still on the market at least half a year after the promotion ended. The analysis took into account monthly sales of a car model before, during, and after a price-promotion activity was conducted (See Figure 2).
Method
To compare the effects of different price-promotion activities, the relative sales impact of each activity needed to be calculated. For this, the time periods in which each promotion category activity was conducted were compared with the time periods before the promotion—i.e., the comparison period.
Thus, instead of analyzing absolute sales figures, the analysis considered category market share—in other words, a product's sales in relation to the total sales of its category (e.g., Audi A4, BMW 3 Series, Mercedes C-Class)—as this figure is not affected by seasonal changes.
The relative sales impact (dependent variable) of each case, therefore, was the change in category market share between the promotion period and the comparison period. Furthermore, this variable was corrected for the occurrence of
push advertising (whether or not the product was also advertised at that time);
competitors' market launches;
the occurrence of multiple promotions (whether or not other promotions were also conducted at that time).
Moreover, because the analyzed manufacturer did not allow disclosure of the actual sales impact figures, standardized data were used.
Data preprocessing yielded a dataset with 203 distinct price-promotion activities. Testing for significant sales-impact differences between promotion types was based on a 2 (price reduction: direct, indirect) × 2 (precondition: without, with) analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with, as fixed factors,
price reduction (direct versus indirect)
precondition (without precondition versus with precondition).
Covariates included in the analysis were promotion intensity (percentage discount) and penetration (cars sold with and without the promotion). The former was used to control for differences in discount rates between different cars and promotion activities.
Controlling for penetration ensured that differences in number of car sales eligible for a promotion activity were taken into account, because some promotion types had certain restrictions, such as a loyalty promotion, which only was applicable for existing customers.
The Results
The two-way ANCOVA results revealed an interaction effect that is significant only at the 0.1 level (F = 3.33, p = 0.07, η2 = 0.02). The condition of direct- versus indirect-price reduction showed a significant direct effect (F = 23.17, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.11). Precondition, however, did not directly influence sales (F = 0.02, p = 0.88, η2 = 0).
Observation of the individual contrasts (See Table 1) lent further support to the interaction effect. Regardless of whether or not a price-promotion activity was tied to a precondition, activities with direct-price reduction yielded higher sales than activities with indirect-price reduction.
The existence of a precondition attenuated the sales impact of direct-price reduction (See Figure 3); moreover, the sales impact of indirect-price reduction was enhanced by the introduction of a precondition.
Discussion of Study 1
The results of Study 1 supported Hypotheses 1, 2, and 3:
Direct-price reduction was found to have a stronger sales impact than indirect-price reduction, supporting H1.
The introduction of a precondition was found to reduce the sales impact of direct-price reduction but not for indirect-price reduction, supporting H2.
The directness of the price reduction was found to have a stronger impact on sales than the existence of a precondition, supporting H3.
It is notable, however, that for indirect-price reduction the existence of a precondition was found to enhance the sales impact. This was in contrast to the assumption that because of the more difficult quantification of the time and effort required for meeting a precondition, no effect would be observed. One possible explanation for this finding would be that consumers, in fact, appreciate limited eligibility of indirect-price reduction. This result is noteworthy and requires further investigation.
STUDY 2: BRAND PERCEPTION
Study Design
Complementing the analyzed sales impacts, Study 2 investigated the effects of different price-promotion activities on consumers' brand perception. Because changes in brand perception of an established brand, caused by a one-time event, may appear only in small units, pretests were performed to ensure adequate measurement.
The pretests showed that a large and intuitive scale to measure the dependent variables was necessary to capture small changes in brand perception. Further, to measure a change in perception, the data collection needed to be conducted in multiple steps, with several days in-between each session.
A two-step study was used to collect data of consumers' brand perception with and without a price promotion. Respondents participated in two separate online surveys:
In the first survey they were faced with a premium car purchase situation, shown an advertising video for a specific model, and asked to rate their perception of the car brand. The dependent variables assessed in the survey were brand image (Low and Lamb, 2000) and prestige (Baek et al., 2010); (See Appendix B, p. 283). Each dependent variable was measured by multiple items on a semantic differential scale. Participants responded to each item on a sliding 50-point scale.
The study's second step was initiated three days after completion of the first survey. Respondents randomly were assigned to one of the four experimental conditions representing the four price-promotion categories. They again were faced with the same purchase situation, but this time a price-promotion activity was advertised. The advertised car was the same that was analyzed in the first survey and was held constant across all experimental conditions. Additionally, the magnitude of the price promotion was held constant across groups. Subsequently, the dependent variables were assessed again.
A sample of 554 respondents was recruited through a market-research firm's online panel. Quota sampling was used for the sample to be representative to German premium-car customers regarding gender (male = 80 percent) and age (25–74 years; M = 51). Furthermore, only subjects who had purchased at least one car in the past were accepted to participate in the survey.
To ensure that the change in perception between the first and second step was caused by the stimulus of a price promotion and not an external factor, a control group with 25 respondents and no manipulation additionally was employed to the described sample.
Two types of analyses were conducted for hypotheses testing:
The change in brand perception (H4) was investigated with a within-subjects multivariate repeated-measures technique. The dependent variables were brand image and prestige. The change in each dependent variable was reported for all four price-promotion categories.
To analyze the different effects between price-promotion types (H5–H8), a 2 (price reduction: direct, indirect) × 2 (precondition: without, with) multivariate between-subjects ANCOVA was used. In this approach the differences between the first and second measure constituted the dependent variables. In all analyses, consideration of whether or not a respondent owned the focal car brand was used as a covariate, to control for potential confounding effects.
Results
Investigation of the change of brand perception (brand image and prestige) between the first and the second survey was conducted using repeated measures.
For the control group, no significant differences could be observed. Respondents who were exposed to a price-promotion manipulation did show significant brand perception changes for specific price-promotion types (See Table 2), whereas
“direct-price reduction without a precondition” (Type 1) and “indirect-price reduction with a precondition” (Type 4) did not show significant changes on brand perception, and
“direct-price reduction with a precondition” (Type 3) and “indirect-price reduction without a precondition” (Type 2) had a negative impact.
Data analysis to test for distinctive impacts of price-promotion types was based on the differences between respondents' brand
Furthermore, the analysis revealed a significant disordinal interaction effect between price reduction and precondition (F = 11.08, p < 0.01, η = 0.02), for both dependent variables,
brand image (F = 18.56, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.03) and
prestige (F = 9.39, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.02).
As this effect interfered with both direct effects, neither price reduction (p > 0.25) nor precondition (p > 0.39) had any direct effects. “Direct-price reduction without a precondition” (Type 1) was significantly less detrimental for brand image than (See Table 3)
“indirect-price reduction without a precondition” (Type 2) and
“direct-price reduction with a precondition” (Type 3).
Accordingly (See Figure 4),
“Indirect-price reduction without a precondition” (Type 2) and “direct-price reduction with a precondition” (Type 3) were significantly more detrimental to brand image than “indirect-price reduction with a precondition” (Type 4);
The individual contrasts regarding prestige showed that “indirect-price reduction without a precondition” (Type 2) was significantly worse for brand prestige than “direct-price reduction without a precondition” (Type 1) or “indirect-price reduction with a precondition” (Type 4).
View this table:“Direct-price reduction with a precondition” (Type 3) did not show a significant difference to another promotion regarding prestige.
Discussion of Study 2
There were two steps in Study 2.
In the first step, the authors analyzed change in perception of brand image and prestige between the first and the second survey. The results showed that “indirect-price reduction without a precondition” (Type 2) and “direct-price reduction with a precondition” (Type 3) led to a significant decline in brand image and prestige perception, lending partial support to H4.
However, neither
“direct-price reduction without a precondition” (Type 1) nor
“indirect-price reduction with a precondition” (Type 4)
was found to negatively affect brand perception. Although this finding was in contrast to this study's hypothesized general negative effect of price-promotion activities for brand perceptions, the pattern is similar to that of Study 1:
“Direct-price reduction without a precondition” and
“indirect-price reduction with a precondition”
were more beneficial for sales—and not harmful for the brand.
By contrast, price-promotion activities that allow for a direct quantification of the precondition or that offer additional products of features without a limited eligibility were less beneficial for sales and were detrimental for the brand.
In a second step, the authors compared four different types of price promotion. The outcome supported six hypotheses but rejected two:
H5a and H6a were supported. The negative effect on brand image was found to be lower for “direct-price reduction without a precondition” (Type 1) than for “indirect-price reduction without a precondition” (2) and “direct-price reduction with a precondition” (3).
H7a and H8a were supported, assuming “indirect-price reduction with a precondition” (Type 4) to have a lower negative impact on brand image than “indirect-price reduction without a precondition” (2) and “direct-price reduction with a precondition” (3).
Regarding the effects on the dependent variable, prestige:
H5b and H7b were supported. “Direct-price reduction without a precondition” (Type 1) and “indirect-price reduction with a precondition” (4) were found to have a lower negative impact than “indirect-price reduction without a precondition” (2) but not than “direct-price reduction with a precondition” (3)—rejecting H6b and H8b.
The results showed that when comparing the four price-promotion types, “direct-price reduction with a precondition” (Type 3) had the most detrimental effects on brand image, whereas “indirect-price reduction without a precondition” (2) had the most detrimental effects on prestige.
CONCLUSION
Across two studies, the research team investigated sales impact as well as the brand perception impact of different types of price-promotion activities in the premium automobile market. The results revealed that a direct-price reduction has a stronger positive sales impact than activities with an indirect-price reduction, in line with earlier findings (Alvarez and Casielles, 2005).
Additionally, the sales increase achieved by direct-price reduction was found to be attenuated when a precondition was introduced. Limiting a promotion activity's eligibility through a precondition, however, was found to even enhance sales for indirect-price reduction.
Regarding the brand impact, promotion activities that are more common (i.e., familiar to the consumer) were found to constitute less negative perceptual change. Compared with more-common promotion types, “direct-price reduction with a precondition” (Type 3) was found to be most detrimental for brand image.
Combining the sales and brand-perception impact, “direct-price reduction without a precondition” (Type 1) appeared to be the ideal price-promotion type for premium products, as it had the strongest positive influence on sales and no negative impact on brand image and prestige. As the theoretical approach of prospect theory presumed (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979), a promotion that is designed to lower the loss of the transaction is perceived as stronger than a promotion that extends the gain of the product.
Moreover, a promotion type that is more common creates less negative associations than a more unusual promotion type, which was assumed based on the Covariation principle of Attribution Theory (Kelley, 1973; Mizerski et al., 1979). These theories accordingly proposed the outcome that “indirect-price reduction without a precondition” (Type 2) has the lowest positive sales impact and at the same time a significant negative impact on brand image and prestige.
IMPLICATIONS
Overall, the results of Study 1 and Study 2 offer a more differentiated picture of price-promotion activities than most existing research, the authors of this article found.
Specifically, marketers and advertisers carefully should consider how a price-promotion activity is framed with regard to the directness of the price reduction and the existence of a precondition, as both aspects were found to significantly influence promotion effectiveness.
One managerial takeaway is, therefore, that marketers could focus on price-promotion activities with direct-price reduction when their goal is to increase sales. Preconditions, such as a limited eligibility, however, should be considered only in conjunction with indirect-price reductions that extend the value of the promoted product.
Moreover, promotion types that are well-known among customers should be preferred to minimize a premium brand's image and prestige deterioration. The often newly created promotion types that advertisers use to hide a discount may not have the intended success but rather negate the purpose of the action.
LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE RESEARCH
Several limitations should be considered when interpreting the results.
As real-market data was analyzed in Study 1, not all interfering variables could be controlled for. For example, no information about competitor promotion campaigns was available.
The structure of the actual market data did not allow for analyzing the general sales impact of price promotion. The results thus are limited to a comparison between different price-promotion types.
Regarding the detrimental effect of unusual price-promotion activities on brand perception, another relevant aspect could be the frequency with which a brand applies common or unusual price promotions, as consumers could use this variable to judge about a premium brand's inability to sell at the regular price.
How the different price-promotion types influence other perceptions of the brand—perceived quality or exclusivity, for instance—should be analyzed in future studies. Additionally, a further differentiation between consumers should create further insights on the impact of price promotion. As some of the analyzed promotion types may be somewhat specific for the automobile industry, the generalizability of the results needs to be considered closely. Indeed, the perception of a brand usually changes in the long run. It would be interesting to initiate a longitudinal study to analyze the brand-perception change over time.
Furthermore, as the current research focused on premium automobiles, it is uncertain how the findings would change if other product categories, such as nondurable premium products, were analyzed. The authors, therefore, suggest that further research compare the impact of price promotion for durable and nondurable premium products.
ABOUT THE AUTHORS
Felix Zoellner is pricing specialist at BMW Group's price- and volume-planning department in Munich, Germany. His research specialty is pricing and brand perception. Before his career at BMW, Zoellner was a doctoral student at the EBS University, Wiesbaden, Germany.
Tobias Schaefers is assistant professor at TU Dortmund University, Germany. His research interests include social-media marketing, access-based services, sponsorship, and pricing. Schaefers's work has been presented at international conferences and published in several journals, including the Journal of Interactive Marketing, Marketing Letters, Psychology & Marketing, the International Journal of Advertising, and the European Journal of Marketing.
APPENDIX A
APPENDIX B
Footnotes
↵1 “Chrysler Woos Volkswagen Owners With Cash-Back Offer,” The Wall Street Journal, December 6, 2012.
- © Copyright 2015 The ARF. All rights reserved.