Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current issue
    • Archive
    • Digital First
    • JAR Best Paper
    • Gold Open Access
  • Topics
    • All topics
    • Celebrity endorsement
    • Mobile
    • Multicultural
    • Neuromarketing
    • Pharmaceutical and Healthcare
    • Shopper insights
    • Social media
    • Sponsorship
    • Television
  • Submit
    • Calls for Papers
    • Submit a manuscript
  • Subscribe
    • Subscribe to JAR
    • Pay per view
    • ARF members
  • About
    • About JAR
    • Impact factor
    • Meet the Editors
    • ARF and Editorial Review
    • JAR in the News
    • My Folders
    • Feedback
    • Contact
  • Other Publications
    • jadvertres

User menu

  • Subscribe
  • Contact Us

Search

  • Advanced search
  • Other Publications
    • jadvertres

Log in

  • Facebook
  • Twitter
  • Linkedin
the Journal of Advertising Research

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current issue
    • Archive
    • Digital First
    • JAR Best Paper
    • Gold Open Access
  • Topics
    • All topics
    • Celebrity endorsement
    • Mobile
    • Multicultural
    • Neuromarketing
    • Pharmaceutical and Healthcare
    • Shopper insights
    • Social media
    • Sponsorship
    • Television
  • Submit
    • Calls for Papers
    • Submit a manuscript
  • Subscribe
    • Subscribe to JAR
    • Pay per view
    • ARF members
  • About
    • About JAR
    • Impact factor
    • Meet the Editors
    • ARF and Editorial Review
    • JAR in the News
    • My Folders
    • Feedback
    • Contact

How Do Heuristics Influence Creative Decisions at Advertising Agencies?

Factors that Affect Managerial Decision Making When Choosing Ideas to Show the Client

Douglas C. West, George Christodoulides, Jennifer Bonhomme
DOI: 10.2501/JAR-2017-056 Published 1 June 2018
Douglas C. West
King's College London,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: douglas.west@kcl.ac.uk
George Christodoulides
Birkbeck, University of London,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: g.christodoulides@bbk.ac.uk
Jennifer Bonhomme
Young & Rubicam, New York,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: Jennifer.I.bonhomme@gmail.com
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & Data

Tables

  • TABLE 1

    Selected Empirical Research on Heuristics and Decision-Making Performance

    Study and ContextResearch GoalMain Findings
    Survey of 93 MBA students and 185 marketing managers in the U.S. (Hoch, 1988).Ability of experts to predict the opinions of consumers compared with novices, with neither group having access to data.Experts were no better than novices at using their intuition. Wrong decisions involving intuition cannot be subject to feedback.
    Survey of 16 academics, 12 practitioners, and 43 high school students in the U.S. (Armstrong, 1991).Differences between predictions of experts and novices.No significant differences between any groups.
    Two experiments with 114 and 59 MBA students in the U.S. (Cripps and Meyer, 1994).Investigation of how consumers plan for the replacement of durable goods compared with optimal machine-replacement theory.Subjects made persistently suboptimal decisions, based upon a conservative heuristic, favoring obsolescence-motivated replacement of durables than better performing alternatives.
    Two experiments with 220 students and volunteers in Germany (Bröder, 2003).Is the use of heuristics in decision making adaptive, and does information acquisition correspond to decision strategies?Choosing the appropriate heuristic requires a meta-heuristic that integrates and evaluates cues from the environment that convey information about its payoff structure. Saving costs in the long run were traded off against not always choosing the best option, for example.
    Three experiments with 52 students and faculty in the U.K. (Newell and Shanks, 2003).Assessment of the parameters of the take-the-best (TTB) heuristic by offering the purchase of more information before the choice is finalized.Complex decisions processes can be performed in simple ways. Although TTB is powerful, it is not the universal decision-making tool.
    Survey of 561 projects containing 499 failures and 62 commercial successes in 1989–1994, including 1,143 entrepreneurs in Canada (Åstebro and Elhedhli, 2006).Understanding why Canadian Invention Assistance Program analysts correctly forecast the likelihood that an invention would reach the market as often as or more often than linear additive statistical models.Analysts use simple sums of counts, using significantly more cues than typically observed. The conjunctive model predicted 86 percent. Experts, however, correctly predicted 83 percent, significantly outperforming a log-linear additive statistical model (79 percent).
    Three experiments with 497 students in the U.S. (Saini and Monga, 2008).Investigation of whether consumer decision making is more heuristic when it comes to spending time rather than money.Time and money might be seen as the same by economists, but in practice, consumers more likely will use heuristics to reduce time, whereas they more likely will use algorithms when it comes to spending money.
    Analysis of three datasets including information on 2,330 apparel, 2,891 airline, and 2,357 CD customers in the U.S. (Wübben and Wangenheim, 2008).Comparison of the outcomes of stochastic models versus the heuristics used by firms to predict future purchases.Complex methods were only slightly superior to heuristics in terms of determining the (in)activity of customers, and there was no clear evidence that such models are superior to heuristics.
    Three experiments with 530 undergraduates in the U.S. (Hutchinson, Alba, and Eisenstein, 2010).Differences between optimal marketing-budget allocations and those predicted by heuristics.Data-based inferences are subject to strong, heuristic-based biases that are not reduced by graphical presentations of the data, “real world” experience, or explicit training.
  • TABLE 2

    Heuristic Types Presented in the Survey

    Typology LabelDescription of Decision Tools
    AlgorithmicThe creative work that proved best on the basis of analyzing the data
    DefaultThe creative work most similar to what we normally choose to do
    DeferThe creative work that we thought the client wanted
    EqualityWe didn't make one choice; we integrated creative works equally from all competing campaigns
    ExperienceThe creative work that the most experienced person in our team wanted
    FluencyThe creative work we recognized quickest
    HierarchyThe creative work that senior agency managers wanted
    InstinctWe followed our instincts
    MajorityThe creative work most people wanted
    RecognitionThe creative work we most easily recognized
    SatisficingThe first creative work that exceeded our objectives
    Take the bestThe creative work we thought would be best for the client
    TallyingThe creative work with the highest number of favorable aspects to it
  • TABLE 3

    Demographic Characteristics of the Sample

    Demographicn (%)
    Age of office (n = 95)
    M106
    Mdn136
    Mode150
    Staff
    No. years at agency (n = 88)
    M3
    Mdn2
    Mode1
    Age (n = 90)
    25–3445 (50)
    35–4426 (29)
    18–2410 (11)
    45–547 (8)
    55–642 (2)
    Gender (n = 89)
    Female43 (48)
    Male46 (52)
    Job (n = 112)
    Account planner/researcher21 (18.8)
    Account director12 (10.7)
    Digital account director11 (9.8)
    Digital media11 (9.8)
    Digital account planner/researcher11 (9.8)
    Community manager6 (5.4)
    Creative director5 (4.5)
    Copywriter/art director3 (2.7)
    SEO specialist3 (2.7)
    Designer/specialist3 (2.7)
    Digital copywriter/art director2 (1.8)
    Digital creative director1 (0.9)
    Media0 (0.0)
    Other23 (20.5)
    • Note: SEO = search engine optimization.

  • TABLE 4

    Decision-Making Techniques and Confidence

    HeuristicAgreement (scored 1–7)
    MSD
    Take the best: The creative work we thought would be best for the client or sponsor5.751.34
    Tallying: The creative work with the highest number of favorable points about it4.701.87
    Instinct: We followed our instincts.4.571.82
    Satisficing: The first creative work that exceeded our objectives; we then ignored the rest.4.022.03
    Majority: The creative work most people wanted3.931.84
    Algorithmic: The creative work that proved best on the basis of analysis of the data3.871.97
    Defer: The creative work the client wanted3.911.76
    Experience: The creative work that the most experienced person in our team wanted3.361.85
    Hierarchy: We chose the creative work that senior managers wanted.2.951.77
    Recognition: The creative work we most recognized2.861.70
    Default: The creative work most similar to what we normally choose to do2.841.53
    Equality: We didn't make a choice; we allocated resources equally to all competing creative works.2.881.81
    Fluency: The creative work we recognized quickest2.511.57
    • Note: N = 69.

  • TABLE 5

    Heuristic Type Factor Loadings

    ItemComponentCommunalities
    AcknowledgeTopKnow-HowBreakdown
    Recognition.883.813
    Fluency.879.875
    Default.711.602
    Instinct.735.631
    Satisficing.720.546
    Take the best.667.629
    Tallying.521.422
    Experience.832.793
    Hierarchy.821.741
    Defer.516.679
    Equality.834.721
    Algorithmic.657.489
    • Note: Kaiser-Mayer-Olkin = .749, Bartlett's test of sphericity = .000, total variance explained = 65.271.

  • TABLE 6

    Regression Estimates

    ModelBSE Bβtp
    Constant74.8055.20914.359.000
    Heuristic
    Acknowledge−3.9972.545−.192−1.571.123
    Top11.4302.390.5554.784.000
    Know-How0.1272.609.0060.049.961
    Breakdown2.2142.739.1050.808.423
    Controls
    Years in advertising0.4820.697.1750.691.493
    Years in this agency−0.2391.117−.046−0.214.831
    No. agencies−1.8161.920−.179−0.945.349
    Creative/Planner1.2838.294.0200.155.878
    • Note: The dependent variable was confidence. R2 = .374.

  • TABLE 7

    Correlations

    VariableAcknowledgeTopKnow-HowBreakdown
    Age−.253*.173−.169−.248
    Years in this agency−.287*.067−.093−.267*
    No. agencies−.145.087−.111−.265*
    Years in advertising−.185.068−.135−.240
    • Note: * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level.

PreviousNext
Back to top

Vol 58 Issue 2

Journal of Advertising Research: 58 (2)
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • Cover (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word about the Journal of Advertising Research.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
How Do Heuristics Influence Creative Decisions at Advertising Agencies?
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from the Journal of Advertising Research
(Your Name) thought you would like to see this page from the Journal of Advertising Research web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
Alerts
Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
Citation Tools
How Do Heuristics Influence Creative Decisions at Advertising Agencies?
Douglas C. West, George Christodoulides, Jennifer Bonhomme
Journal of Advertising Research Jun 2018, 58 (2) 189-201; DOI: 10.2501/JAR-2017-056

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Download PDF
Request Permissions
Share
How Do Heuristics Influence Creative Decisions at Advertising Agencies?
Douglas C. West, George Christodoulides, Jennifer Bonhomme
Journal of Advertising Research Jun 2018, 58 (2) 189-201; DOI: 10.2501/JAR-2017-056
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Save to my folders

Jump to

  • Article
    • ABSTRACT
    • MANAGEMENT SLANT
    • INTRODUCTION
    • CREATIVITY AND CREATIVE CHOICES
    • METHODOLOGY
    • RESULTS
    • DISCUSSION
    • CONCLUSION, LIMITATIONS, AND FUTURE RESEARCH
    • ABOUT THE AUTHORS
    • REFERENCES
  • Figures & Data
  • Info
  • PDF
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

More in this TOC Section

  • An Investigation of Androgyny and Sexual Orientation in Advertising
  • Are Brands Wasting Money On Sport Sponsorships?
  • How Anthropomorphized Brand Spokescharacters Affect Consumer Perceptions and Judgments
Show more Articles

© 2021 the Journal of Advertising Research

The ARF is the premier advertising industry association for creating, aggregating, synthesising and sharing the knowledge required by decision makers to lead and succeed.

www.thearf.org

JAR is published four times a year for the Advertising Research Foundation by WARC. Both subscribers and ARF members can access recent issues of JAR via this site.

A larger JAR archive is accessible at WARC, alongside case studies, best practice guides, marketing intelligence, consumer insight, industry trends and latest news from around the world.

Take a trial of WARC.com

Navigate

  • Home
  • Current Issue
  • Archive
  • Digital First
  • JAR Best Paper
  • Topics
  • Submit a manuscript
  • Calls for Papers
  • About JAR
  • Subscribe to JAR
  • Pay per view
  • ARF members
  • Meet the Editors
  • ARF and Editorial Review
  • JAR in the News
  • My Folders

Contact us

  • Contact
  • Feedback
  • ARF members

General

  • About the ARF
  • About WARC
  • Rights & Permissions
  • Advertise in JAR
  • Terms of Use